Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Self-Efficacy & Outcome Expectations in Career Development: Social Cognitive Theory, Study notes of Psychology

Counseling PsychologyCareer CounselingSocial PsychologyVocational Psychology

How Social Cognitive Theory, specifically self-efficacy and outcome expectancies, influences career interests, choices, and performance. The theory, proposed by Albert Bandura, suggests that self-efficacy comes from previous accomplishments, observing others, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. The constructs of self-efficacy and outcome expectancies are an individual's perceptions of reality, which may or may not be realistic. Hackett and Betz were the first to apply this theory to career choices, and Lent et al. developed a social cognitive framework to explain and predict career behavior.

What you will learn

  • How do self-efficacy, outcome expectancies, and interests influence career choices?
  • How does self-efficacy develop according to Social Cognitive Theory?
  • What is the difference between self-efficacy and outcome expectancies in Social Cognitive Theory?

Typology: Study notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/12/2022

larryp
larryp 🇺🇸

4.8

(33)

353 documents

1 / 7

Toggle sidebar

Related documents


Partial preview of the text

Download Self-Efficacy & Outcome Expectations in Career Development: Social Cognitive Theory and more Study notes Psychology in PDF only on Docsity! 10   Social  Cognitive  Career  Theory   Introductory  Review   Written  and  provided  by  Nadya  A.  Fouad   From  Career  Theory  and  Practice:  Learning  Through  Case  Studies  Third  Edition,  2014         The  theoretical  approaches  we  have  examined  thus  far  have  been  well-­‐established   theories  that  have  shaped  vocational  psychology  for  several  decades.  However,  the  theory   we  discuss  in  this  chapter  was  developed  about  two  decades  ago.  The  concepts  were  first   introduced  in  1981  (Hackett  &  Betz,  1981),  and  a  complete  description  of  the  theory  wasn’t   published  until  1994  (Lent,  Brown,  &  Hackett,  1994).  We  include  this  theory  because  of  its   utility  and  because  of  the  impact  this  theory  has  had  on  the  field.  It  differs  from  the  other   theories  in  its  focus  on  the  personal  constructions  that  people  place  on  events  related  to   career  decision  making  (Lent,  Brown,  &  Hackett,  2002;  Lent,  2013).     Social  cognitive  theory  (Bandura,  1977,  1986,  1997)  has  been  recently  applied  in   vocational  psychology  to  help  explain  how  individuals’  career  interests  develop,  how  they   make  career  choices,  and  how  they  determine  their  level  of  performance.  Bandura   hypothesizes  that  individuals’  conception  of  their  confidence  to  perform  tasks  (self-­‐ efficacy)  mediates  between  what  they  know  and  how  they  act  and  that  people’s  beliefs  in   their  ability  to  accomplish  things  helps  to  determine  the  actions  they  will  take.  Self-­‐efficacy   comes  from  individuals’  previous  performance  accomplishments,  vicariously  by  observing   others,  from  verbal  persuasion,  and  from  physiological  states  and  arousal.     Bandura  also  postulates  that  self-­‐efficacy  is  distinct  from  outcome  expectancies,  or   the  expectations  one  has  of  the  result  of  behavior.  Bandura  (1986)  notes  that  self-­‐efficacy   and  “outcome  expectancies  judgments  are  differentiated  because  individuals  can  believe   that  a  particular  course  of  action  will  produce  certain  outcomes,  but  they  do  not  act  on  that   outcome  belief  because  they  question  whether  they  can  actually  execute  the  necessary   activities”  (p.  392).  The  constructs  of  self-­‐efficacy  and  outcome  expectancies  are  an   individual’s  perceptions  of  reality;  as  such,  those  perceptions  may  or  may  not  be  realistic.  It   is  important  to  note  that  in  decision  making,  individuals’  perceptions  of  reality  are   hypothesized  to  be  greater  determinants  of  their  behavior  than  objective  reality.     Hackett  and  Betz  (1981)  were  the  first  to  apply  Bandura’s  social  cognitive  theory,   with  its  emphasis  on  the  role  of  self-­‐efficacy,  to  career  choices.  They  focused  on  self-­‐efficacy   theory  to  explain  women’s  traditional  career  choices,  suggesting  that  low  self-­‐efficacy  may   explain  the  restricted  range  of  women’s  career  options.  Their  work  led  to  investigations  of   the  role  self-­‐efficacy  may  play  in  a  variety  of  career-­‐related  behaviors,  and  the  constructs  of   self-­‐efficacy  and  outcome  expectancies  were  then  incorporated  into  a  theoretical   framework  related  to  career  decisions.  Lent  et  al.  (1994,  2002)  developed  a  social  cognitive   framework  to  explain  and  predict  career  behavior.  Specifically,  their  three-­‐part  model  links   interests,  choices,  and  performance  based  on  Bandura’s  social  cognitive  model.     Basic  to  all  three  of  the  segments  of  the  model,  Lent  et  al.  (1994,  2002;  Lent,  2013)   propose  that  performance  accomplishments,  verbal  persuasion,  vicarious  learning,  and   physiological  states  and  arousal  forge  an  individual’s  self-­‐efficacy  expectations.  For   example,  a  young  woman  who  does  well  in  French  class,  is  persuaded  by  others  that  she   could  be  mistaken  for  a  native  French  speaker,  observes  others  speaking  French,  and  is   mildly  anxious  about  performing  well  would  be  expected  to  have  high  self-­‐efficacy  beliefs   for  speaking  French.  It  is  important  to  note  that  Bandura’s  conceptualization  of  self-­‐efficacy   is  situation-­‐specific.  In  the  example  above,  the  young  woman  may  have  high  self-­‐efficacy  for   speaking  French  but  lower  self-­‐efficacy  beliefs  for  speaking  German  or  Russian.   Lent  et  al.  (1994,  2002;  Lent,  2013)  also  propose  that  demographic  and  individual   difference  variables  (such  as  sex,  race/ethnicity,  and  socioeconomic  status)  interact  with   background  and  contextual  variables  to  influence  learning  experiences  that  play  a  role  in   forming  self-­‐efficacy  beliefs.  Those  self-­‐efficacy  expectations,  in  turn,  are  related  to   outcome  expectations  that  individuals  have  about  the  outcomes  of  behavior.  In  the  earlier   example  of  the  young,  French-­‐speaking  woman,  a  high  socioeconomic  status  may  have   provided  her  with  opportunities  to  learn  French  and  may  lead  to  her  outcome  expectations   of  speaking  French,  such  as  opportunities  to  travel  or  live  in  France.     In  the  interest  segment  (see  Figure  10.1),  outcome  expectancies  and  self-­‐efficacy   beliefs  both  predict  interests  (Lent,  2013).  Interests  (together  with  self-­‐efficacy  beliefs  and   outcome  expectancies)  predict  goals,  which  in  turn  lead  to  behaviors  related  to  choosing     The  choice  model  (Lent  et  al.,  2002;  Lent,    2013)  proposes  that  person  inputs  (e.g.,   gender,  race,  disability,  personality,  and  predispositions)  and  background  context  together   influence  learning  experiences,  which  influence  self-­‐efficacy  beliefs  and  outcome   expectancies  (see  Figure  10.2).  As  already  described,  these  influence  interests,  which   influence  choice  goals;  goals  influence  actions,  and  actions  influence  performance   attainments.  For  example,  a  young  girl  from  an  affluent  background  is  taken  to  science   museums,  encouraged  to  read  and  learn  about  science  and  famous  scientists,  and  given   opportunities  to  take  science  classes  and  to  attend  summer  science  camp.  These  learning   experiences,  afforded  by  her  socioeconomic  status,  influence  the  development  of  her  beliefs   in  her  ability  to  do  well  in  science.  Her  performance  in  science  and  her  knowledge  that   doing  well  in  science  will  lead  to  positive  outcomes,  such  as  good  grades,  parental  approval,   and  time  spent  with  friends,  lead  to  the  development  of  her  interest  in  science.  She  believes   she  can  do  well  in  science  in  college,  she  learns  that  science  is  a  field  that  is  well   compensated  and  one  that  is  not  typical  for  women,  and  she  develops  an  intention  to  enter   a  science  major  in  college.  Lent  (2013)  notes  that  the  process  of  making  a  career  choice   involves  choosing  a  goal  (e.g.,  becoming  a  scientist),  taking  action  to  implement  that  goal   (completing  courses  in  a  biology  major),  and  the  subsequent  consequences  of  those  actions   (successful  graduation  in  biology).     The  performance  model  predicts  the  level  of  performance  as  well  as  the  persistence   an  individual  has  in  pursuing  goals  (Lent  et  al.,  2002;  Lent,  2013).  This  segment  (see  Figure   10.3)  proposes  that  past  performance  accomplishments  influence  self-­‐efficacy  and  outcome   expectancies,  which  in  turn  influence  performance  goals;  these  lead  to  performance   attainment  level.  In  other  words,  past  performance  influences  self-­‐efficacy  beliefs  along   with  the  expectations  individuals  have  about  the  outcomes  of  their  future  behavior.  These   expectations  affect  the  goals  that  people  set  for  themselves.  These  goals  then  affect  the   level  of  performance  they  may  attain.  Thus,  a  woman  who  did  well  in  basketball  while  in   high  school  has  confidence  in  her  ability  to  play  and  decides  to  try  out  for  the  basketball   team  in  college.  After  making  the  team,  she  sets  challenging  goals  for  her  offensive  and   defensive  performance  (e.g.,  averaging  at  least  12  points  a  game),  based  on  her  past   successes  and  current  self-­‐efficacy  and  outcome  beliefs.  Lent  et  al.  differentiate  their  choice   and  interest  models  from  the  performance  model.  The  former  involve  the  content  of  career   choices,  such  as  the  field  or  specific  occupation  in  which  one  would  like  to  work,  while  the   performance  model  predicts  the  level  of  performance  toward  which  one  aspires  within   one’s  chosen  field.   -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐   Figure  10.3:    Predicting  Task  Performance  in  Social  Cognitive  Career  Theory   -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐     Bandura  (1997)  comments  that  “in  making  career  decisions,  people  must  come  to   grips  with  uncertainties  about  their  capabilities,  the  stability  of  their  interests, . . . the   prospects  of  alternative  occupations, . . . and  the  type  of  identity  they  seek  to  construct  for   themselves”  (p.  422).  Individuals’  perceptions  of  their  own  efficacy  in  mastering  various   skills  and  tasks  play  an  important  role  in  predicting  their  choices  as  well  as  their   perseverance  to  accomplish  their  goals.  Social  cognitive  theory  thus  is  readily  applicable  to   development  of  interventions  targeted  at  increasing  individuals’  self-­‐efficacy  in  a  variety  of   areas  (Betz  &  Schifano,  2000).  Other  areas  for  intervention  include  promoting  aspirations   and  interests  in  children  and  adolescents,    expanding  career  choice  options,  fostering   positive  and  realistic  outcome  expectations,  setting  specific  goals,  coping  with  barriers  and   building  supports,  and  increasing  coping  self-­‐efficacy  and  strong  performance  skills  (Lent,   2013).  Interventions  may  be  targeted  at  expanding  vocational  interests,  at  increasing   decision-­‐making  skills  and  exploratory  behavior,  at  helping  clients  explore  various  careers,   and  at  increasing  consideration  of  nontraditional  careers.   influence self-efficacy and outcome expectancies, which in turn influence performance goals; these lead to performance attainment level. In other words, past perf rmance influences s lf-efficacy beliefs along with the expectations individuals have about the outcomes of their future behavior. These expectations affect the goals that people set for themselves. These goals then affect the level of performance they may attain. Thus, a woman who did well in basketball while in high school has confidence in her ability to play and decides to try out for the basketball team in college. After making the team, she sets challenging goals for her offensive and defensive performance (e.g., averaging at least 12 points a game), based on her past successes and curre t self- fficacy and outcome beliefs. Lent et al. differentiate their choice and interest models from the performance model. The former involve the content of career choices, such as the field or specific occupation in which one would like to work, while the performance model predicts the level of performance toward which one aspires within one’s chosen field. Bandura (1997) comments that “in making career deci ions, people must come to grips with uncertainties about their capabilities, the stabil- ity of their interests, . . . the prospects of alternative occupations, . . . and the type of identity they seek to construct for themselves” (p. 422). Individuals’ perceptions of their own efficacy in mastering various skills and tasks play an important role in predicting their choices as well as their perseverance to accomplish their goals. Social cognitive theory thus is readily applicable to development of interventions targeted at increasing individuals’ self-efficacy in a variety of areas (Betz & Schifano, 2000). Other areas for intervention include expanding career choice options, fostering positive and realistic outcome expectations, setting specific goals, coping with barriers and building supports, and increasing coping self- efficacy and strong performance skills. Interventions may be targeted at expanding vocational interests, at increasing decision-making skills and exploratory behavior, at helping clients explore various careers, and at increasing consideration of nontraditional careers. Social Cognitive Career Theory 191 Task Performance Self-Efficacy Ability/ Past Performance Outcome Expectations Performance Goals/Subgoals Performance Attainment Level Figure 10.3 Predicting Task Performance in Social Cognitive Career Theory SOURCE: Lent, Brown, and Hackett (1994).   The  social  cognitive  career  model  has  generated  a  great  deal  of  interest  among   researchers  in  vocational  psychology.  Roughly  20%  of  the  articles  published  in  vocational   psychology  in  the  past  10  years  have  emphasized  some  aspect  of  the  social  cognitive  career   theory  (SCCT).  Research  has  demonstrated  support  for  the  relationship  between  self-­‐ efficacy  and  interests,  between  self-­‐efficacy  and  outcome  expectations,  and  the  influence  of   the  model  in  predicting  occupational  choices  (Betz,  2008;  Juntenun  &  Even,  2013;  Lent,   2013;  Lent  et  al.,  2002).  Donnay  and  Borgen  (1999)  demonstrated  that  self-­‐efficacy  helps   to  explain  vocational  choice  over  and  above  vocational  interests,  and  Sheu,  Lent,  Brown,   Miller,  Hennessy  and  Duffy  (2010)  found  that  self-­‐efficacy,  interests,  and  outcome   expectations  predicted  choices  across  Holland  themes  .  Finally,  performance   accomplishments  have  been  shown  to  be  the  most  powerful  influence  on  self-­‐efficacy   (Williams  &  Subich,  2006).  Research  on  SCCT  has  also  supported  the  model  across   racial/ethnic  groups,  across  many  developmental  levels,  and  for  both  men  and  women   (Fouad  &  Kantamneni,  2013).  Betz  concludes  her  review  of  the  research  on  SCCT  by  noting   that  “the  theory  has  relevance  for  understanding  a  wide  range  of  vocational  behaviors   relevant  to  both  career  choice  and  adjustment”  (p.  365).