Download Literature Review in Thinking Skills, Technology and Learning - Book Summary - English literature and more Summaries English Literature in PDF only on Docsity! Literature Review in Thinking Skills, Technology and Learning REPORT 2: FUTURELAB SERIES Rupert Wegerif, School of Education, Open University AIMS AND PLAN The purpose of this report is: • to clarify what is meant by thinking skills and their relationship to technology • to identify the role of ICT in promoting thinking skills • to produce guidelines for the development of digital learning resources to support the teaching and learning of thinking skills • to evaluate the general direction of research in this area and how this should inform educational practice. The use of new technologies is often linked to the development of thinking skills or ‘higher order thinking’. This review will explore some of the claims that have been made in this area and summarise the useful findings that emerge from research. There is a range of different approaches to understanding thinking skills and learning, each one of which has an impact on how the relationship between thinking skills and technology is conceptualised. The first part of the review explores the literature about teaching thinking skills and their relationship to technology. The second part focuses more on technology, exploring claims that have been made about the relationship of information and communications technologies to the development of thinking skills. 1 CONTENTS: EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 2 SECTION 1 TERMS AND CONTEXT 4 SECTION 2 CAN WE TEACH THINKING SKILLS? 12 SECTION 3 TEACHING THINKING SKILLS WITH TECHNOLOGY 20 SECTION 4 IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE, CURRICULUM DEVELOPMENT AND THE DESIGN OF LEARNING RESOURCES 34 GLOSSARY 37 INTERNET LINKS 37 BIBLIOGRAPHY 39 Literature Review in Thinking Skills, Technology and Learning REPORT 2: FUTURELAB SERIES Rupert Wegerif, School of Education Open University This report has been designed to enable both rapid identification of the key findings and in-depth exploration of the literature. The key findings and implications of the report are presented within the Executive Summary and Implications Sections. The main body of the review enables readers to explore in more detail the background to these headline issues. HOW CAN WE DESIGN TECHNOLOGY TO SUPPORT TEACHING THINKING SKILLS? The finding that collaboration enhances the learning of thinking skills is important because most software is still designed for individual work. There are several simple design guidelines that could be applied to develop software to support more collaboration (see Section 3.5.3 for details). Some of the findings about effective teaching for thinking skills could also be incorporated into software design. For example, being explicit about thinking skills, modelling them, designing activities that use the same skills in different contexts and prompting learners to reflect on thinking strategies and articulate them clearly. Three ways in which the use of ICT can particularly enhance the teaching and learning of thinking skills emerged from the review: Firstly, through supporting dynamic and multiple representations of information: Visualising patterns in data-sets, for example, allows learners to think at a higher level about statistical relationships. Secondly, through a certain ambivalence: educational software can act like a teacher to prompt and direct enquiry but can, at the same time, act as a resource while learners discuss and explore ideas. This makes properly designed educational software an effective way of supporting thinking within the curriculum. An example of this productive ambivalence could be to prompt reflection (directive teaching) before, during and after the use of a simulation (discovery learning). Thirdly, networks can allow students to engage directly in knowledge creation with others who are not physically present. Given the apparent importance of collaborative learning this has significance for home education. Depending on how the activity is arranged, thinking together with others at a distance can be more motivating and can stimulate a higher quality of thought, than thinking together with others in the same classroom. The best software for teaching thinking skills stems from collaborations between developers and educators or educational researchers. 1 TERMS AND CONTEXT The main terms of this review all have multiple and disputed meanings. I do not intend to use definitions of these terms as a limit to the scope of the review but as a starting-off point to further exploration. 1.1 THINKING SKILLS Thinking Skills now feature in the National Curriculum for England where they are described as follows: By using thinking skills pupils can focus on ‘knowing how’ as well as ‘knowing what’ – learning how to learn. The following thinking skills complement the key skills and are embedded in the National Curriculum: Information-processing skills These enable pupils to locate and collect relevant information, to sort, classify, sequence, compare and contrast, and to analyse part/whole relationships. 4 the best software for teaching thinking skills stems from collaborations between developers and educators or educational researchers SECTION 1 TERMS AND CONTEXT Reasoning skills These enable pupils to give reasons for opinions and actions, to draw inferences and make deductions, to use precise language to explain what they think, and to make judgements and decisions informed by reasons or evidence. Enquiry skills These enable pupils to ask relevant questions, to pose and define problems, to plan what to do and how to research, to predict outcomes and anticipate consequences, and to test conclusions and improve ideas. Creative thinking skills These enable pupils to generate and extend ideas, to suggest hypotheses, to apply imagination, and to look for alternative innovative outcomes. Evaluation skills These enable pupils to evaluate information, to judge the value of what they read, hear and do, to develop criteria for judging the value of their own and others’ work or ideas, and to have confidence in their judgements. (DfES 2002a http://www.nc.uk.net/ learn_think.html) This definition has the advantage of being clear. However it misses out some aspects of thinking skills that most practitioners and experts in the area agree are important. The most authoritative definition of critical thinking is that of ‘The Delphi Report’ (Facione, 1990) which is a consensus report from 46 leading experts in the field. This emphasises the holistic nature of critical thinking, the importance of cultivating dispositions and the social context of critical thinking. The executive summary runs to 20 pages and offers 14 useful recommendations. The shortest form of their definition is given below. We understand critical thinking to be purposeful, self-regulatory judgment which results in interpretation, analysis, evaluation, and inference, as well as explanation of the evidential, conceptual, methodological, criteriological, or contextual considerations upon which that judgment is based. CT is essential as a tool of inquiry. As such, CT is a liberating force in education and a powerful resource in one's personal and civic life. While not synonymous with good thinking, CT is a pervasive and self-rectifying human phenomenon. The ideal critical thinker is habitually inquisitive, well-informed, trustful of reason, open-minded, flexible, fair-minded in evaluation, honest in facing personal biases, prudent in making judgments, willing to reconsider, clear about issues, orderly in complex matters, diligent in seeking relevant information, reasonable in the selection of criteria, focused in inquiry, and persistent in seeking results which are as precise as the subject and the circumstances of inquiry permit. Thus, educating good critical thinkers means working toward this ideal. It combines developing CT skills with nurturing those dispositions which consistently yield useful insights and which are the basis of a rational and democratic society. (Facione, 1990) Philosopher Richard Paul is often described as the leading proponent of teaching critical thinking (e.g. Weinstein, 1993). He would accept the definition above but he also goes further in wanting to add a focus on fostering dialogue. Paul argues 5 for ‘strong’ critical thinking, it is important to question one’s own assumptions through thinking from the perspectives of others that, for ‘strong’ critical thinking, it is important to question one’s own assumptions through thinking from the perspectives of others. ‘Meta-cognition’ is another term often used as a synonym for thinking skills or higher order thinking. This has been defined as ‘awareness of one’s own knowledge and the ability to understand, control and manipulate individual cognitive processes’ (Osman and Hannafin, 1992 p83). Meta-cognition originates in an information processing model of the mind as something like a computer running both low-level software, to do the basic cognitive processes, and high level software, to monitor and correct the low level software. However this term is increasingly used in contexts where a computer model of the mind is not being assumed. It is often used to refer to any conscious reflection on thinking and learning processes. Some ‘skills’, habits and dispositions important to good thinking in many contexts are not meta- cognition in this sense. Therefore I believe that the term ‘meta-cognition’ does not refer to all thinking skills but to that sub-set of such skills that depend upon becoming more aware of thinking processes. Sharon Bailin (1998) opposes the use of the term ‘skills’ on the grounds that its use in psychology leads to it being taken to imply a property of the brain. She argues that critical thinking is essentially a normative and not a descriptive term. She means by this that critical thinking is not merely a description of how we think but is concerned with how we think well. More precisely, it is about the quality of reasoned judgements, and this can be assessed by shared criteria. Building a bridge that collapses will involve most of the same cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies, and activate the same regions of the brain in the engineers, as building a bridge that stands. But we want to promote the skills that went into building the bridge that stands. Thinking skills are therefore not just about abstract cognitive processes – they are about the quality of socially embedded decision making processes. However Carol McGuinness argues in favour of retaining the term ‘thinking skills’: …the idea of thinking-as-a-skill continues to have both theoretical and instructional force. Firstly it places thinking firmly on the side of "knowing how" rather than "knowing that" in the long standing philosophical debate about the nature of knowing. And secondly much of what we know about skill learning can be usefully applied to developing thinking… (1998, p4/5) McGuinness goes on to mention how well the ‘skills’ terminology fits with the increasing importance of ideas of apprenticeship to teaching and learning. I agree with Bailin that understanding and promoting good thinking requires working with shared criteria for the evaluation of arguments. However I also agree with Carol McGuinness that the idea of thinking-as-a-skill is a useful one for practitioners. In everyday language to describe someone as skilled at something - say at ballet dancing or wood-carving - implies a public performance to which shared criteria can be applied. There is no need to assume a more specialist meaning for the term ‘skills’. Use of the term ‘thinking skills’ might also 6 SECTION 1 TERMS AND CONTEXT In the 1996 Handbook of Educational Psychology, James Greeno, Allan Collins and Lauren Resnick, three very authoritative names in the field of educational research, define learning and transfer as follows: Learning is the process by which knowledge is increased or modified. Transfer is the process of applying knowledge in new situations. Educators want the knowledge that is acquired in school to apply generally in students’ lives, rather than being limited to the situations in classrooms where it is acquired. That is to say, they want the knowledge to transfer. I accept this definition of learning. It has the advantage of being very broad. Greeno, Collins and Resnick argue that there are three main schools of learning theory in educational psychology: 9 Four orientations to learning (after Merriam and Caffarella, 1991, p 138) ASPECT BEHAVIOURIST COGNITIVIST/ HUMANIST PARTICIPATORY CONSTRUCTIVIST EDUCATOR'S ROLE ARRANGES ENVIRONMENT TO ELICIT DESIRED RESPONSE STRUCTURES CONTENT OF LEARNING ACTIVITY FACILITATES DEVELOPMENT OF THE WHOLE PERSON WORKS TO ESTABLISH COMMUNITIES OF PRACTICE IN WHICH CONVERSATION AND PARTICIPATION CAN OCCUR VIEW OF THE LEARNING PROCESS CHANGE IN BEHAVIOUR INTERNAL MENTAL PROCESS INCLUDING INSIGHT, INFORMATION PROCESSING, MEMORY, PERCEPTION A PERSONAL ACT TO FULFIL POTENTIAL INTERACTION /OBSERVATION IN SOCIAL CONTEXTS. MOVEMENT FROM THE PERIPHERY TO THE CENTRE OF A COMMUNITY OF PRACTICE LEARNING THEORISTS THORNDIKE, PAVLOV, WATSON, TOLMAN, SKINNER, SUPPES PIAGET, AUSUBEL, BRUNER, PAPERT MASLOW, ROGERS LAVE, WENGER, COLE, WERTSCH, ENGESTROM LOCUS OF LEARNING STIMULI IN EXTERNAL ENVIRONMENT INTERNAL COGNITIVE STRUCTURING AFFECTIVE AND COGNITIVE NEEDS LEARNING IS IN RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PEOPLE AND ENVIRONMENT VIEW OF TRANSFER COMMON ELEMENTS SHARED BY DIFFERENT CONTEXTS OVER-ARCHING GENERAL PRINCIPLES CHANGES IN SELF- IDENTITY AS A LEARNER TRANSFER PROBLEMATIC PURPOSE IN EDUCATION PRODUCE BEHAVIOURAL CHANGE IN DESIRED DIRECTION DEVELOP CAPACITY AND SKILLS TO LEARN BETTER BECOME SELF- ACTUALIZED, AUTONOMOUS FULL PARTICIPATION IN COMMUNITIES OF PRACTICE AND UTILIZATION OF RESOURCES • behaviourist/empiricist: learning as acquiring and applying associations • cognitivist/rationalist: learning as acquiring and using conceptual and cognitive structures, and • situative/pragmatist-sociohistoric: learning as becoming attuned to constraints and affordances through participation. These three schools are a version of three of the four traditions in the table above. In their detailed account of these three different approaches to understanding learning and transfer Greeno, Collins and Resnick demonstrate that each tradition has made a valuable contribution to understanding some aspects of learning. Behaviourist approaches have worked well for some basic skills learning, the cognitivist tradition has done some good work on meta-cognition and conditions of transfer while the social situationists have shown the importance of cultural tool- systems and communities of learning. However each approach also has weaknesses and this is an area of study that is open and constantly evolving. I will return to this account of the main divisions in educational psychology in part three of this report in order to consider the impact of each of these three approaches on ways of thinking about the role of new technology in learning. 1.4 THINKING SKILLS, TECHNOLOGY AND LEARNING IN CONTEXT Thinking skills are often said to be the kind of skills that people need to make decisions (e.g. Ennis, 1996). There used to be many fewer decisions for most people to make. Plato, for example, advocated teaching reason only to the small elite who would have to take all the major decisions, (and even then only when they were over thirty!) See Plato’s Republic, (1970). The majority of the population of his ideal ‘Republic’ would be given a more vocational education fitted to their station. Plato saw little point in equipping them with skills that they would not have an opportunity to use. Plato’s point makes some sense in the economic circumstances of Ancient Greece and a similar principle was applied until quite recently in the UK. One point of agreement running through almost every article that I have read on teaching thinking skills is that the need to teach thinking skills now is rooted in our particular socio-historical situation. Thinking skills are everywhere described as ‘skills for the new century’, ‘skills for the workers of the future’ or ‘skills for the knowledge society’. Basically the argument is that changes in the economy require more people to be more actively involved in decision making than was ever the case before. It is usually also claimed that these changes in the economy are driven by the development of new information and communication technologies. The thinking skills literature is full of references to the need to produce new knowledge workers for the new knowledge economies (e.g Swartz, 2001). One idea behind this is that new technologies have led to increasing automation of the kinds of work that computers can do thereby forcing people into jobs where they have to take more subtle decisions and solve more complex problems (Levin and Rumberger 1995 quoted in Rassool, 1999 p153). A second key idea is that new technology in the work place has led to rapid and accelerating 10 thinking skills are often said to be the kind of skills that people need to make decisions SECTION 1 TERMS AND CONTEXT changes in practices and that this puts a premium on learning how to learn, since anything more specific that children are taught in school is seen as likely to be out of date by the time they leave. The aim of this review is to explore the relationship between technology and thinking skills. One dimension of this relationship is that the kinds of thinking that people value most depend on the cultural and historical context and particularly upon the kind of technology that people have at their disposal to help them think. The Ancient Greeks had very simple technologies and they valued that kind of thinking that distinguished them most from the animals around them. Aristotle defined man as a rational animal meaning that only man could measure, judge and decide on the basis of reasons (Aristotle, 1987). Before the arrival of computers in human history it seemed natural to many to describe ‘higher order thinking’, or rationality, in terms of abstract reason on the model of formal logic or mathematics. This kind of thinking was really hard, potentially very useful and only a few people could do it well. Computers, however, find formal reasoning very easy. What they find hard is the sort of things most people take for granted like coming up creatively with new ways forward in complex, fast-changing and open-ended contexts where there is no certainty of being right. Holding an ordinary conversation is typical of what it is that humans find natural but computers find extremely hard. It is not surprising therefore that, as the use of computer-based technology has become more ubiquitous, the focus of thinking skills research has shifted away from the sort of things that computers can do for us, such as formal reasoning or algorithmic problem solving, towards the sort of things that computers cannot yet do. Instead of contrasting human thinking to the thinking of animals, human thinking is beginning to be contrasted to the thinking of machines (e.g Penrose, 1994: Hobson, 2002). The focus of published thinking skills research is shifting away from teaching logic and towards a greater interest in supporting complex unpredictable thinking (Resnick, 1987), engagement in dialogues (Paul, 1987), intuition (Claxton, 1999) and creativity (Bailin, 1994). One way in which thinking skills are related to developments in technology is therefore simply that the human skills that we value most, and that are rewarded the most, are those skills that computers cannot yet imitate. 1.5 SUMMARY OF SECTION 1 Thinking skills are hard to define but possible to recognise and to teach. The list of thinking skills in the UK National Curriculum is a reasonable one and similar to many: information-processing, reasoning, enquiry, creative thinking and evaluation. Such skills are not discrete entities but part of larger contexts. Most approaches to teaching thinking skills include broader issues such as engagement in dialogue, the formation of self-identity and the importance of a supportive culture. Technology is a broad term for human tool systems. I will limit it, for the purposes of this review, to computer- based technologies used to handle information and aid communication (ICT). The concept of learning is bound up with the concepts of knowledge and of transfer. 11 maths exam even though, logically, some of the problem-solving and reasoning strategies involved are the same in both cases. A brief summary of research on the use of LOGO given in part 3 of this review shows that this conclusion has been confirmed in practice. However, this does not mean that transfer is impossible – it means that some thought and effort needs to be put into achieving it. If, for example, a video-game involves problem solving strategies, then the nature, scope and limits of the strategies need to be made explicit, and examples need to be offered of the same strategy used in new contexts with new content. Simulations that allow learners to practice these strategies in life beyond video games could be used to help bridge the transfer of general skills from one context to another. 2.3 ARE THINKING SKILLS INDIVIDUAL OR SOCIAL? Thinking skills are often seen as attributes of individuals, perhaps the property of individual brains. However there has been a major shift in both philosophy and psychology towards seeing thought as essentially social not individual. The increasing status given to the ideas of educational psychologist Lev Vygotsky supports this tendency in psychology. Vygotsky, a Marxist, is often presented as providing a psychological version of Marx’s claim that individual thought is a product of the social and historical context (e.g. Edwards, 1996, p43). In particular Vygotsky claims that language is a tool-system that mediates the development of thought. This fits well with research on distributed cognition, which suggests that cognition is not located in individuals so much as in systems that, while they may include people, also include objects and technologies (Salomon, 1993). The roots of critical thinking are not necessarily individualist. John Dewey, an advocate of teaching thinking, saw thinking as a product of social interaction and teaching thinking as a way of contributing to the creation of a better society (Dewey, 1933). Jurgen Habermas (1991), has argued in a similar way that rationality implies the ideal of a more genuinely democratic society in which all relevant voices are really listened to and decisions are taken on the basis of the quality of arguments rather than on the basis of coercive power. One educational implication of Habermas’ argument is that teaching thinking skills involves changing the social context to create conditions that at least approximate to what he calls an ‘ideal speech situation’. Experimental evidence, as well as Vygotskian theory, suggests that the quality of individual thinking reflects the quality of collective thinking and vice versa (Wegerif, Mercer and Dawes, 1999: Mercer, 2000). Growing acceptance of the idea that thinking is embedded in social contexts can be seen reflected in the language used to discuss thinking skills. There is an increasing use of collective terms such as ‘thinking classrooms’ (McGuinness, 1998), ‘thinking schools’ (Wilson, 2000) and ‘communities of inquiry’ (Lipman, 1991). ‘Philosophy for Children’, a popular approach to teaching thinking skills, already works primarily by drawing children into discussions and turning the classroom into a community that supports thinking (Fisher, 1998). The new field of the study of consciousness might have something interesting to say about this issue. 14 thinking skills are often seen as attributes of individuals, perhaps the property of individual brains. However there has been a major shift in both philosophy and psychology towards seeing thought as essentially social not individual SECTION 2 CAN WE OR SHOULD WE TEACH THINKING SKILLS? Consciousness is clearly important to many approaches to higher order thinking, in particular the idea of using meta- cognitive strategies to become more aware of one’s own thinking. In some ways consciousness is inescapably an individual property. But interestingly the word is formed from Latin roots meaning ‘knowledge with another’. One implication from the work of Hobson (2002: 1998) quoted earlier, as well as other approaches to understanding consciousness, is that individual self- consciousness is formed out of internalised dialogue. In other words to be aware of something at all is to see it as if from the perspective of other people and so to be potentially able to express it and to share it with other people. From this and other sources (e.g Wittgenstein’s private language argument, 1967) it seems that there are good reasons to think that both self-consciousness and higher order thinking are social in origin. In other words thinking skills originate in conversations where we learn to reason, to evaluate, to join in creative play and to provide relevant information. However, at the same time it is clear that all social thinking could be seen as being made up of individual contributions and is rooted in potentially measurable individual brain processes. There is a constant movement of the internalisation of external thinking into individual thinking and externalisation out again by individuals who create and shape social thinking. To understand this vision of thinking as a whole flowing movement it might help to think of the way that creative speakers and writers (i.e. all of us) both use words and phrases that they find already there, external to them, in the language and yet also shape the development of that shared resource of language. One important conclusion from this debate is that higher order thinking is to be found in the whole circular movement of thinking and not just in the most private individual moment of it. In the past thinking skills interventions have focussed on supporting the internal moment of the circular movement of thinking and the internalisation process. However, if higher order thinking is to be seen in the whole movement, then it follows that it is may also be valuable to improve the quality of social thinking and the quality of the externalisation process. Technology, in various forms from language to the internet, carries the external form of thinking. Technology therefore has a role to play through supporting improved social thinking (eg providing systems to mediate decision making and collective reasoning) and also through providing tools to help individuals externalise their thinking and so to shape their own social worlds. 2.4 ARE CRITICAL THINKING SKILLS WHITE, MALE, AND MIDDLE-CLASS? Some argue that critical thinking skills are not really of general value but are a product of the experience of a particular social group usually described as white, male and professional or managerial. Ruqaya Hasan, for example, in a study that shows that there is more language of explicit problem solving and reasoning in the homes of managers and professionals than in blue collar homes, claims that the value given to this way of using language reflects Marx’s claim that ‘in every age the ideas of the rulers are the ruling ideas’ (Marx, 1977 quoted in Hasan 1992). Harvey Siegel (1987) and Sharon Bailin (1998) respond to this kind of challenge 15 in other words thinking skills originate in conversations where we learn to reason, to evaluate, to join in creative play and to provide relevant information with the argument that criticisms of critical thinking already imply critical thinking and can only be assessed through the giving and evaluating of reasons. However this focus on explicit reasoning fails to fully address some of the concerns expressed. Feminist philosopher, Seyla Benhabid (1992) seems to be have more insight into why certain groups feel excluded by the ideal of critical thinking and offers what I think is a constructive way forward. Her argument, based upon Habermas, is that, if we are to bring up children in peace we actually do need some sort of ideal of reason or at least an ideal of ‘being reasonable’. This ideal is about how real people solve their problems without resorting to violence through engaging in dialogues informed by an attitude of care and respect. There is a view that emerges from the literature that this ‘concrete ideal’ of dialogue across difference is the origin of ‘higher order thinking’. For Richard Paul, as was noted earlier, critical thinking in the strong sense has to be ‘dialogical’. What he means by this is that the critical thinker has an obligation to question his or her own assumptions in order to try to understand the perspective of others. If, as Seyla Benhabid suggests, reason is situated in real dialogues then, in the course of such dialogues, assumptions about what constitutes good reasoning will themselves have to be questioned and allowed to evolve. If there are different ways of thinking with something to offer – and claims have been made for the value of more intuitive and holistic feminine and non-western ways of thinking - then this should emerge in such genuine ‘strong’ critical thinking dialogues. But any such evolution of our understanding of higher order thinking would only serve to strengthen the process of higher order thinking itself, if this is understood as essentially and originally ‘dialogue across difference’ (Burbules and Rice, 1991). There is already evidence that access to technology at home enhances educational achievement (Reeves, 1998) and it is acknowledged that access to ICT in homes is patterned along socio-economic lines. If teaching thinking equips children for individual success in a competitive market place and no more than that, then teaching thinking with computers could prove to be another way to reproduce social inequality. It may well be that defining thinking skills in a narrow way often reflects the experience and self-interest of a particular social group. On the other hand, if, as the evidence suggests, higher order thinking has a social side as well as an individual side, then teaching thinking requires the promotion of more intelligent forms of collective thinking. Some writers, indeed, have brought out the connection between teaching thinking skills with ICT and the promotion of an intelligent and responsive global democracy (e.g Cobb, 2002: Rassool, 1998). 2.5 SHOULD THINKING SKILLS BE TAUGHT SEPARATELY FROM CONTENT AREAS? Different positions in the debate about the nature of thinking skills suggest different responses to the question of how to teach thinking skills. Belief in what has been called ‘the central processor model’ of the mind tends to suggest that teaching thinking skills directly in a separate programme will automatically have a general impact. There are many such 16 defining thinking skills in a narrow way often reflects the experience and self-interest of a particular social group SECTION 2 CAN WE OR SHOULD WE TEACH THINKING SKILLS? CASE (Cognitive Acceleration through Science Education) is one of the most successful and well-evaluated programmes in the UK. Incorporating discussion about science problems in classroom, CASE is directed towards scientific reasoning for 11-14 year-olds. CASE succeeded in raising pupils’ grades in GCSE examinations (on average one grade) two to three years after the programme had been completed (Adey and Shayer, 1993, 1994). Recent extension of the CASE programme into Key Stage 1 has also produced evidence of impact on children’s learning (Adey, Robertson and Venville, 2002). A recent meta-analysis of the impact of Feurstein’s Instrumental Enrichment found that overall, the programme had a positive impact (Romney and Samuels, 2001). A total of 40 controlled studies, comprising 47 different samples, were examined. Significant, though modest, average effect sizes were found in all three areas - achievement, ability, and behaviour - with the most extensive improvement being made in ability. Gains in spatial/perceptual ability were related to the length of the intervention (number of hours), and the impact on self-esteem was related to age. Marzano (1998) analysed 4000 intervention studies in education involving over 1,237,000 subjects. Marzano found that nearly all interventions worked to some extent but that interventions that focussed on the level of meta-cognition, (ie teaching thinking and learning strategies), and the level he called the self-system (ie how students feel about themselves as learners) were most effective in improving measures of learning. Marzano’s findings strongly support the success of teaching thinking skills if we translate this to mean the teaching of meta-cognitive strategies. Marzano writes that: Specifically, instructional techniques that employed the metacognitive system had strong effects whether they were intended to enhance the knowledge domains, the mental process within the cognitive system, the beliefs and processes within the self-system, or the processes within the metacognitive system itself. Similarly, teaching that specifically focused on emotions and sense of identity (the self system) had a powerful effect on learning gains at every level. Implicit in this finding is a transfer effect from teaching focussing on attitudes and feelings to gains in learning measures. Hattie, Biggs and Purdie (1996) conducted a meta-analysis of 51 study skills interventions. They found that ‘Despite, perhaps, the conventional wisdom, most intervention does work most of the time’ (1996, p128). However separate general study skills programmes were found to be much less effective than teaching meta- cognitive strategies as part of the teaching of content within courses. The statistical evidence now seems convincing. But another kind of evidence that should not be ignored is the evidence of experience. We all have experience as both teachers and learners. We all know intuitively that it is possible for learning to change people and to become part of them. We also know that it is possible to learn skills that are used outside the context in which they were taught. That is how we can think at all when faced with a 19 separate general study skills programmes were found to be much less effective than teaching meta- cognitive strategies as part of the teaching of content within courses completely new challenge. The fact that these things sometimes prove hard to evaluate in a rigorous way should not lead us to deny such obvious and everyday experience. It is intuition based upon this kind of experience that lies behind the interest of so many teachers in teaching thinking skills. In my view that intuition is not misguided. 2.8 SUMMARY OF SECTION 2 The existence and nature of thinking skills is contested. Few experts in the field would now support the claim that there are universal thinking skills or completely general strategies for learning and problem solving. However there is consensus that there are a range of relatively general learning strategies that can be disembedded from some contexts and re-embedded again in new contexts. Thinking is indissoluably both individual and social. There is a constant movement of the internalisation of social thinking into individual thinking and externalisation out again by individuals into social thinking. Higher order thinking is to be found in the whole flowing circular movement of thinking and not just in the individual moment of it. Technology in various forms, from language to the internet, carries social thinking. Technology therefore obviously has a role to play in supporting higher order thinking. Many have criticised specific ideals of higher order thinking and thinking skills as being too western, masculine and middle class. However the ideal of being able to listen seriously and empathetically to challenges and to respond to reasonable challenges with reform is intrinsic to the ideal of higher order thinking. This kind of challenge is therefore not really an attack– it is a valuable part of the process of higher order thinking. The consensus, supported by research evidence, is that the best way to teach thinking skills is not as a separate subject but through ‘infusing’ thinking skills into the teaching of content areas. Brain research is not yet in the position to act as an authoritative support for different teaching and learning strategies. However research findings so far have been suggestive and may well lead to more clear evidence in the future. Of particular interest is the possibility of finding objective correlates for previously rather vague sounding notions such as the expansion of consciousness. There have been several rigorous surveys of the impact of different teaching methods and programmes in the last decade. These provide convincing evidence for the value of explicitly teaching transferable thinking skills. 3 TEACHING THINKING SKILLS WITH TECHNOLOGY 3.1 CONCEPTUALISATIONS OF THE ROLE OF ICT IN EDUCATION Theories of how the mind works have had a strong effect on the way that the role of new technology has been thought about in education. Surveys of the use of computers to promote thinking skills by both Hughes (1990) and by Underwood and Underwood (1990) draw a sharp distinction between the use of computers as a tutor to teach thinking skills and the use of computers as a tool in order to develop skills indirectly. Crook (1994), in a similar survey, argues 20 convincing evidence for the value of explicitly teaching transferable thinking skills SECTION 3 TEACHING THINKING SKILLS WITH TECHNOLOGY that both these ways of conceptualising the role of the computer in relation to thinking skills are inadequate. He develops a third approach which he refers to as the use of computers as a ‘mediational means’ to ‘resource collaborative encounters’ (Crook, 1994 p227). These three conceptualisations, computer as tutor, computer as tool or computer as support for dialogue, are reflections of the three main traditions in educational psychology referred to by Greeno, Collins and Resnick and quoted in the first part of this review. I will briefly consider each in turn. 3.1.1 THE COMPUTER AS TUTOR The behaviourist tradition does not have much to say directly about thinking skills. The language of the movement is mostly about producing observable changes in behaviour. To educate effectively on this model requires a great deal of contingent individual re-inforcement. Computers can provide the individual feedback needed. Skinner was influential in promoting the use of computers as individual teaching machines. Instructional technologists broke down complex tasks into learning hierarchies in order to produce teaching programmes. The current use of Integrated Learning Systems (ILSs) continues in the tradition of the computer as an individual teaching machine. There is evidence that this individualised approach leads to improved learning of some basic skills (Underwood et al 1996). The main criticism of the computer as a tutor model is that directed computer teaching does not allow children to be creative learners, able to think and make connections for themselves, and so is unlikely to support the development of higher order thinking (e.g. Papert, 1981: Underwood and Underwood, 1990) This criticism assumes constructivism: the claim that knowledge is the product of an active process of construction rather than a passive assimilation of information. 3.1.2 THE COMPUTER AS ‘MINDTOOL’ Several popular Science Fiction films show characters ‘downloading’ skills directly into their brains from computers. The implication is that human skills and computer programmes are very similar. Richard Clark refers to the cause of this popular linking of computers and thinking skills as the reification of a metaphor: the computer metaphor of mind popular in cognitive science (Clark, 1990, p268). When the mind is seen as a kind of computer it seems plausible that working with computers can provide mind skills. Seymour Papert, a student of Piaget, applied constructivism to the role of computers (1981, 1993) advocating the use of programming and other active modelling environments to support learning (where learning was seen as the active construction of meaning). Constructivism is now probably the dominant paradigm in the design of educational multimedia (Boyle, 1996, p83). The implicit idea in much of the literature of cognitivism and constructivism is that the mind is so much like a computer that teaching a computer how to think, i.e. programming, is pretty much the same as learning how to think for oneself. Jonassen (2000) outlines the significance of cognitive psychology and constructivism for the use of technology to promote the development of thinking skills. Mindtools are computer applications that, when used by learners to represent what they know, necessarily engage them in 21 problem, planning, and the kind of diagnostic thinking involved in debugging • abilities of formal reasoning and representation eg thinking of all possible combinations, and constructing mathematical models • cognitive styles eg precision, and reflectivity over impulsivity • enthusiasms and tolerances eg persistence, and enthusiasm for meaningful academic engagement. The logic based programming language LOGO has been widely used in schools and widely evaluated. Results seem equivocal. Simon (1987) surveys a number of evaluations to conclude that Papert’s hopes that using LOGO would lead to the emergence of general problem solving skills were ‘pipe dreams’ and ‘techno- romanticism’. Underwood and Underwood’s survey of evaluation results is much more positive (1990). Liao and Bright (1991, quoted in Kirkwood, 1998) conducted a meta-analysis of sixty-five classroom based studies into the relationship between computer programming and general cognitive skills, using quantitative comparisons between treatment and control groups. Their main conclusion is that programming can provide a mildly effective approach to developing students’ cognitive skills in a classroom setting. Erik De Corte (1990) demonstrated that using LOGO alone does not produce transfer but that LOGO can be a useful resource in teaching approaches that lead to transfer. The same point is made by Clements and Gullo (1984). Hughes (1990) sums up a balanced survey of the evidence with the following conclusions: Exposure to LOGO by itself does not usually lead to cognitive gains; that such gains are more likely to be found with structured teaching; and that the Logo environment promotes social interaction amongst peers. (1990. p133) 3.3.2 VISUALISATIONS AND SIMULATIONS Some kinds of powerful thinking work through a series of ‘leap-frogging’ manoeuvres. Scientific thinking, for example, relies on turning processes into nouns so that they can be objectified and thought about more easily (Halliday and Martin, 1993). Representations of every kind allow us to objectify our thoughts so that we can reflect upon them. Writing, graphs, tables and specialist notations such as mathematics are already cognitive tools allowing thought to ‘leap-frog’ to a higher level of understanding. Computers can take this further through allowing the direct manipulation of representations. Paul Cobb argues that providing computer tools to help students manipulate complex data-sets enables them to understand statistical arguments and therefore equips them to be able to participate in many public debates (2002). Sharon Ainsworth (1997) similarly argues for the value of multiple representations for supporting the development of understanding. Jonassen makes the same kind of case for the use of ‘visualisation tools’ that allow learners to visualise scientific ideas (Jonassen, 2000). Many simulations of systems play a similar role. They allow users to manipulate dynamic representations of real-world systems. The literature about using the computer as a tool sometimes blurs the distinction 24 using a computer as a ‘mindtool’ to solve a problem and learning how to solve it for oneself are therefore different things SECTION 3 TEACHING THINKING SKILLS WITH TECHNOLOGY between using external cognitive tools, eg computers, and developing internal cognitive tools, eg thinking skills. These are not the same things. I use a calculator when I change currency, perhaps it is a ‘cognitive tool’, but that does not mean that I learn how to do long division – I can do long division only because a teacher taught me a pencil and paper technique that I still remember and use. When asked to do long division in the absence of paper and pencil I find myself visualising the numbers and imagining the procedure. I do not find it useful to visualise a calculator and imagine pressing the buttons. If I were a hospital manager funding the development of a computer system to diagnose patients I would want the best system possible, a system that did not need doctors. If, on the other hand, I wanted to help teach medical students the complex skills of diagnosis, involving reasoning, evaluation, information processing and so on, I would want a very different system. I would want the careful ‘scaffolding’ of skills with the rich presentation of case studies and lots of interactive feedback. Using a computer as a ‘mindtool’ to solve a problem and learning how to solve it for oneself are therefore different things. Jonassen claims that the best way to learn about an area is to build a computer system to model the area. From my experience of building computer systems I suspect that in reality far more time would be spent learning how to get the computer tools you were using to do exactly what you wanted than in learning about the area. This is not a very efficient approach to teaching and learning. The evidence suggests that learning skills that can transfer requires someone, usually a teacher, to plan activities and experiences that help to make it happen. There is no good evidence that thinking skills will simply ‘rub off’ as a ‘cognitive residue’ (Salomon 1991 quoted in Jonassen, 2000), from using new technology. The point about ‘mindtools’, as Jonassen calls them, is that they do not work on their own. To learn from such tools students need to be primed as to what to look for, they need opportunities to articulate what they find and they need feedback on their discoveries. One way of achieving this is to use mindtools as a resource for small group collaborative learning within a teaching and learning framework (Laurillard, 1993 p137: Wegerif, 2002). 3.3.3 CONCEPT MAPPING Concept maps or ‘semantic networks’ are spatial representations of the concepts and their interrelationships that are intended to represent the knowledge structures that humans store in their minds (Jonassen, 2000). While concept maps do not require computers, computer-based concept- mapping software, such as ‘SemNet’, ‘Learning Tool’, ‘Inspiration’, ‘Mind Mapper’, and many others, enable the production of concept maps. Great claims are made for the use of concept-mapping as a tool to support critical thinking and reflection on the organisation of knowledge in a subject area while also learning about the area (Buzan and Buzan, 2000: Jonassen, 2000). The purpose of semantic networks is to represent the structure of knowledge that someone has constructed. So, creating semantic networks requires learners to analyze the structural relationships among 25 the point about ‘mindtools’, as Jonassen calls them, is that they do not work on their own. To learn from such tools students need to be primed as to what to look for, they need opportunities to articulate what they find and they need feedback on their discoveries the content they are studying. By comparing semantic networks created at different points in time, they can also be used as evaluation tools for assessing changes in thinking by learners. (Jonassen 1998) Concept maps have been used to support inquiry-based science and claims have been made that concept mapping is especially suited to science education. Some research argues that the benefits of concept-mapping can be greatly enhanced if they are used as a focus for collaborative learning (Roth, 1994, Roth and Roychoudhury, 1994) Diana Laurillard (1993, p123), writing in the context of higher education, is critical of the way in which concept maps reduce knowledge to little chunks of information and defined relationships between them. Knowledge, she argues, is unitary and indivisible. ‘even a simple statement such as "as air rises it cools" cannot be expressed as an association between two component fragments’ She is right of course but there is no denying that many people find that concept maps help them to think more clearly about some topics. Educational evaluations of using concept maps mostly seem positive but are small scale (e.g Scanlon et al 1996; van Boxtel et al, 2000). 3.3.4 HYPERTEXT Hypertext is a ‘computer-based software system for organising and storing information to be accessed nonsequentially and constructed collaboratively by authors and users’ (Jonassen, 1991, 83). The world wide web is an example of hypertext. There have been very large claims made for the revolutionary nature of hypertext in education. The non-linearity of hypertext is meant to reflect the way that the mind is structured. Reading hypertext involves making a path through it and so is said to be a more constructive process than reading linear print text. External links made between nodes in the hypertext are said to reflect internal semantic links (Jonassen 1998). A review of research on hypertext use in education by Dillon and Gabard (1998 referred to in Bromme and Stahl, 2002) showed no support for the claim that hypertext aides the teaching and learning of thinking skills. The idea of hypertext is similar to the idea of a library. Those already equipped with effective thinking and learning skills can use libraries as a resource for learning but most students will need more guidance than that. Simply having a library does net mean that you acquire information searching skills. Bromme and Stahl (2002) argue that, while reading hypertext has few learning benefits, constructing hypertexts is likely to involve thinking skills (2002). Their arguments are similar to those put forward for the value of using concept maps, these are that in developing hypertext documents students need to think about the conceptual structure of an area and reflect on the nature of the links between content. 3.3.5 HYPER-MEDIA Hyper-media essentially means hypertext with multi-media content. Constructivists, 26 many people find that concept maps help them to think more clearly about some topics SECTION 3 TEACHING THINKING SKILLS WITH TECHNOLOGY supporting collaborative learning is the view which is most in accord with the sociocultural theoretical framework. Teasley and Roschelle argue that it throws the emphasis away from the computer software and on to the quality of the dialogue. Further evidence that the pedagogy is as important as the technology comes from my own work with Neil Mercer, Lyn Dawes and Karen Littleton at the Open University. In this research an approach has been developed using computers that prepares children to work effectively together with specially-designed computer-based activities focused on curriculum-related topics. A series of 'Talk Lessons' are followed, in which classes establish ground-rules for collaboration such as listening with respect, responding to challenges with reasons, encouraging partners to give their views and trying to reach agreement. These activities are not only concerned with improving the quality of children’s working relationships, but also with developing their use of language as a tool for reasoning and constructing knowledge. That is, the Talk Lessons encourage teachers to create a 'community of enquiry' in their classrooms in which children are guided in their use of language as a tool for both individual reasoning and collaborative problem- solving. Computers are used not only for stimulating effective language use but also for focusing children’s joint activity on curriculum tasks. This embedded and catalytic role for computers in primary education is distinctive (Wegerif, 1996b: Wegerif and Scrimshaw, 1997; Wegerif, Mercer and Dawes, 1998: Wegerif, Sams and Barrett, 2002: Wegerif and Dawes, 2003). In the most recent study of this approach we developed ICT activities in Maths and Science to cover an entire year of the curriculum. Our evaluation demonstrated that computer-based activities can be used to stimulate reasoned discussion and focus this on curriculum-related learning. We used a control study to demonstrate significant gains in scores on curriculum tests in Science and Maths. We also found significant improvements in solving reasoning test problems when working together in small groups. Video analysis has demonstrated a link between solving such problems and use of ‘exploratory talk’ (Wegerif and Mercer, 2000). This effect also transferred to significant improvements in individual scores on Ravens Progressive Matrices tests. Higgins (2001) argues that the findings of this research offer persuasive evidence that, in combination with the right pedagogy, the use of ICT can support the development of transferable thinking skills. 3.4.2 CONFERENCING There are many claims that electronic conferencing can be an effective support for learning thinking skills through collaborative learning. Such claims can be found in Mason (1989), McConnell (1995) and Harasim et al (1995). At least some of the factors claimed to support reasoning relate to the specific way in which the medium supports discourse. For example: • the ease with which it is possible to ‘take the floor’ in a discussion in comparison with face-to-face discussion • the possibility of having several strands of conversation simultaneously supports more meta-cognitive reflection 29 this embedded and catalytic role for computers in primary education is distinctive • the written nature of the dialogue combined with asynchronicity can allow time for refection while maintaining the intrinsic motivation of a conversation. (Wegerif, 1998) These differences between CMC communication and face-to-face communication have been pointed out by David Graddol (1989) and are reiterated by David McConnell (1994) . The conclusion from both these writers appears to be that CMC can support an egalitarian style of communication in which everyone can participate more easily. One possible implication of this is that CMC might be a good medium for establishing what Habermas (1981) calls an 'ideal speech situation': that situation which, through the elimination of all forms of coercion and through ground rules allowing all to speak, best supports the force of good arguments winning out over other, less rational, factors. However, others have claimed, by contrast, that the medium is particularly prone to aggression and irrationality (Siegal,Dubrovsky, Kiesler and McGuire, 1986 quoted in Jonassen, 2000, 265). Laurillard’s claim that ‘the success of the medium is totally dependent on a good moderator’ (1993 p169) is therefore probably a fair assessment of the situation. Links via email or electronic conference with other schools can be used for the joint construction of knowledge including critical questioning and reasoning. An example might be taking a particular topic to research together in order to develop joint multi-media resources on the web. This approach is found in Margaret Riel's 'circles of learning' (Riel, 1996). 3.4.3 THE INTERNET Steve Higgins (2002) draws a distinction between thinking through and thinking with the internet. As an example of the former he suggests 'Newswise', (http://www.dialogueworks.co.uk/) an internet resource aimed at primary and secondary pupils which offers access to appropriate texts for discussion, a forum for exchanging ideas and support for teachers in developing strategies to help engage their pupils in different aspects of the stories and texts. This uses the internet as a medium for exchanging news stories (accessed by teachers and pupils) and exchanging ideas about those stories. The same principle is found in OneWorldTV (http://tv.oneworld.net/) where video news stories are presented and debated by a global audience. By ‘thinking with the internet’ Higgins means using the internet itself as the tool (rather that just the medium of communication). For example using html with hotspots and links to create non- linear texts or pictures and diagrams that can be explored to support teaching and learning. The interactive relationship with information can be used to promote thinking skills. The search engine Kartoo, for example, offers visual results and shows the results of its metasearch with sites being interconnected by keywords. (http://kartoo.com). With the right pedagogy these kind of resources could be used to help to understand classification and to think more about the relationships between categories. 30 with the right pedagogy these kind of resources could be used to help to understand classification and to think more about the relationships between categories SECTION 3 TEACHING THINKING SKILLS WITH TECHNOLOGY 3.4.4 SHARED DATABASES CSILE, which stands for Computer Supported Intentional Learning Environments (Scardemalia et al, 1991: 1994), consists of a number of networked computers in a classroom where a community database is maintained. The database consists of text and graphical notes, all produced by students and accessible through database search procedures. Teachers work with CSILE in different areas of the curriculum. Students are given a question, they have to find information and record it via notes in the database. Other students then comment on the notes and add new notes. Evaluations of learning outcomes in CSILE classrooms are positive and reflect the development of thinking skills, including great comprehension of texts and deeper explanations of processes as well as the development of a more positive self-image as learners. 3.5 HOW TO TEACH THINKING WITH ICT 3.5.1 UNDERSTANDING EVALUATIONS OF THE RESEARCH Reeves, (1998) quotes many positive effects of technology use in education including learning thinking skills. These findings in the USA have been matched more recently in the UK by the DfES Impact2 study (2002b). One problem with these evaluations, and others mentioned in this section of the report, is that all interventions seem to work. Enthusiastic researchers who believe in their proposed solutions can motivate teachers and children regardless of the content of the intervention. More positively, integrating new technology is often a chance for teachers to re-think their approach. There is evidence that new technology often works in this way as a catalyst for change. A particular problem with evaluating the effects of technology is that such effects depend completely on how the technology is used. The quality of an educational activity is not an effect of the software alone but of the software in context. The same piece of software might have quite different effects if used in different pedagogical contexts. Teachers (or parents or moderators) play a crucial role in setting up and guiding any learning experience with ICT. Many claims in this area need to be treated with caution. There are evidently many enthusiasts who love to see children having fun playing with computer-based toys and games and making wonderful multi-media products. The important question is not ‘do they display thinking skills in what they are doing? Of course they do. A better question is: ‘is there any evidence that these skills will transfer in a useful way to other contexts in their lives?’ 3.5.2 THE PEDAGOGICAL CONTEXT The research evidence seems to suggest that transferable thinking skills will not result unless activities are embedded in teaching and learning dialogues, either with a teacher or with other students. In other words the activity, however creative or fun, needs to be framed in such a way that learning goals are made explicit and bridges are built between contexts. This suggests a need for developers to consider the context of use of any ICT and to provide support for using it in a way that will lead to transferable thinking skills. Out of his experiments with different ways 31 the quality of an educational activity is not an effect of the software alone but of the software in context of the activity crucially depends on how it is framed. Learners need to know what the thinking skills are that they are learning and these need to be explicitly modelled, drawn out and re-applied in different contexts. Effective teaching for transferable thinking skills with ICT contains some or all of the following elements: • teaching a ‘thinking’ vocabulary and giving learners an explicit explanation of the thinking skills that they are to be learnt • observing an expert performing the task (modelling) • giving timely feedback on performance (formative assessment) • direct support in the early stages of learning a task (scaffolding) and then a gradual move towards self-regulation and autonomy (teacher fade-out) • the opportunity to articulate thinking strategies and discuss these with other learners (thinking together) • the explicit demonstration of how thinking strategies acquired in one subject area can be used to solve problems in another area (bridging). Another principle to emerge was that collaborative learning improved the effectiveness of most activities. Where tutorial software alone is ineffective for developing thinking skills, tutorial software used as a basis for discussion between learners can be. Computer mind-tools, such as concept maps or programming languages, all appear to be enhanced when used in pairs or groups who are taught to explicitly articulate their strategies as they work together. This is interesting because most software is still designed for individual work, in many cases it could easily be re-designed to support collaboration. Three ways in which the use of ICT could particularly enhance the teaching and learning of thinking skills emerged: First, thinking skills can be developed by supporting dynamic and multiple representations of information. Visualising patterns in data-sets, for example, allows learners to think at a higher level about statistical relationships. Second, through a certain ambivalence of nature, educational software can act like a teacher to prompt and direct enquiry but can, at the same time, act as a resource while learners discuss and explore ideas. Prompting reflection around a simulation could be an example. Third, networks can allow students to engage directly in knowledge creation with others who are not physically present. Given the apparent importance of collaborative learning this has significance for home education. And while it is not intrinsically superior to think together with those outside the classroom, than with those within, it can be more motivating. If those outside the classroom, have a different perspective on issues it can also stimulate more thought. 4 IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE, CURRICULUM DEVELOPMENT AND THE DESIGN OF LEARNING RESOURCES 4.1 IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE The use of technology alone does not lead to transferable thinking skills. The success of any educational activity crucially 34 learners need to know what the thinking skills are that they are learning and these need to be explicitly modelled, drawn out and re-applied in different contexts SECTION 4 IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE, CURRICULUM DEVELOPMENT AND THE DESIGN OF LEARNING RESOURCES depends on how it is framed. Learners need to know what the thinking skills are that they are learning and these need to be explicitly modelled, drawn out and re-applied in different contexts. A simple but effective strategy is to use a three-part structure to ICT activities. The teacher sets up issues and aims at the beginning and then returns to these in a whole group plenary session at the end after group work by the children. The teacher needs to be explicit about thinking skills aims at the beginning of the lesson, modelling the use of the skills being taught, and then these aims need to be visited again at the end of the lesson. In the plenary the teacher needs to draw out what has been learnt and how this could be applied elsewhere. Explicitly linking to further activities that show how similar kinds of thinking skills can be applied in different areas of the curriculum is also a good idea. Explicit links (bridges) to thinking skills need to be made at the beginning and the end of each activity in which the thinking skill is meant to be applied. 4.2 IMPLICATIONS FOR CURRICULUM DEVELOPMENT Effective teaching for transferable thinking skills with ICT contains some or all of the following elements: • teaching a ‘thinking’ vocabulary and giving learners an explicit explanation of the thinking skills that are to be learnt • observing an expert performing the task (modelling) • giving timely feedback on performance (formative assessment) • direct support in the early stages of learning a task (scaffolding) and then a gradual move towards self-regulation and autonomy (teacher fade-out) • the opportunity to articulate thinking strategies and discuss these with other learners (thinking together) • the explicit demonstration of how thinking strategies acquired in one subject area can be used to solve problems in another area (bridging). Collaboration around ICT activities has been shown to have the potential to enhance the learning of transferable thinking skills. However effective collaborative learning needs to be prepared. It helps if learners are explicitly taught how to reason and learn together before they are asked to work collaboratively with ICT. 4.3 IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DESIGN OF LEARNING RESOURCES The best software for teaching thinking skills comes from collaborations between developers and educators or educational researchers. Some of the guidelines for the effective teaching for thinking skills could be incorporated into software design. For example, being explicit about thinking skills, modelling them, designing activities that use the same skills in different context and prompting learners to reflect on thinking strategies and articulate them clearly. Software can be designed to initiate, resource and frame a discussion just as a teacher can, but unlike teachers they are 35 explicit links (bridges) to thinking skills need to be made at the beginning and the end of each activity in which the thinking skill is meant to be applied never judgmental and have infinite patience. In this way learners can construct their own understandings together but in a way that is directed towards curriculum goals by the computer software. An example of this is to incorporate prompts for discussion around a simulation. Before running a simulation the prompt could be: ‘what is your prediction?’ And afterwards: ‘can you explain why you were right/wrong?’ ICT has a particular role to play through supporting dynamic and multiple representations of information. Visualising patterns in data-sets, for example, allows learners to think at a higher level about statistical relationships. Networks can allow students to engage directly in knowledge creation with others who are not physically present. Given the apparent importance of collaborative learning this has significance for home education. And while it is not intrinsically superior to think together with those outside the classroom than with those within, it can be more motivating. If those outside the classroom have a different perspective on issues it can also stimulate more thought. Building up shared data-bases can be an effective way of teaching and applying thinking skills. Links via email or electronic conference with other schools can be used in similar ways for the joint construction of knowledge, including critical questioning and reasoning. These forms of collaborative knowledge- construction could be supported by in-built supports for thinking. Much of the software that has been called ‘mindtools’, such as concept maps and hypertext authoring environments, has potential to support the development of thinking skills with the right pedagogy. The pedagogy needs to frame individual discoveries within teaching and learning conversations. Collaboration between learners, for example, has been shown to enhance the learning of thinking skills using so called mindtools partly because it encourages the explicit articulation of thinking strategies. The finding about the value of collaborative learning is interesting for designers since most software is obviously intended for individual use. There are several simple ways of designing software to support collaboration. • challenges and problems which have meaning for the learner, and which provide a range of alternative choices that are worth discussing. Such challenges should engage the learners with the content of the software rather than its interface • a clear purpose or task which is made evident to the group and which is kept in focus throughout • on-screen talk prompts which ask the pair or group to talk together, remind them to reach agreement and ask for opinions and reasons • resources for discussion, including information on which decisions can be based, and opportunities to review decisions in the light of new information • no features which encourage individuals to take turns, beat the clock or establish competitive ways of working • clicking, multi-choice answers or audio input to minimise typing (unless the learners have been taught keyboarding skills). 36 learners can construct their own understandings together but in a way that is directed towards curriculum goals by the computer software IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE, CURRICULUM DEVELOPMENT AND THE DESIGN OF LEARNING RESOURCES SECTION 4 Teaching Thinking Skills by Kathleen Cotton, (http://www.nwrel.org/scpd/sirs/ 6/cu11.html) is another entry in School Improvement Research Series at Northwestern University with information on a number of approaches. The Information Literacy Place is Sheila Webber and Bill Johnston's collection of useful information literacy resources. Web resources for teachers and pupils Northumberland LEA's Thinking for Learning (http://ngfl.northumberland. gov.uk/) site has teaching thinking resources developed by teachers using many of the strategies in this book. The Thinking Together site (http://www.thinkingtogether.org.uk/) has resources linked to the articles in this review and the book of the same name by Lyn Dawes, Neil Mercer and Rupert Wegerif. Dialogue Works (http://www.dialogueworks. co.uk/) produce Newswise, an on-line resource to promote thinking through news stories and Storywise a handbook by Karen Murris and Joanna Haynes for developing Community of Enquiry with young children. This is the updated version of 'Teaching Philosophy with Picture Books' which inspired us to try out the community of enquiry with younger pupils in schools. There are many other sources of information if you search on the Internet. Try terms like 'critical thinking' (which is often used in the US) as well as 'thinking skills'. There is no guarantee you will find what you want, but a search can provide useful results. BIBLIOGRAPHY Adey, P and Shayer, M (1994). Really Raising Standards: Cognitive intervention and academic achievement London: Routledge. Adey, P and M. Shayer (1993). An exploration of long-term far-transfer effects following an extended intervention programme in the high school science curriculum. Cognition and Instruction. 11(1):1-29. Adey, P, Robertson, A & Venville, G (2002). Effects of a cognitive acceleration programme on Year I pupils, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72: 1-25 Ainsworth, S (1997). Information technology and multiple representations: new opportunities - new problems. Journal of Information Technology for Teacher Education 6: 93-106. Aristotle (1987). Nicomachean Ethics, trans. J.A.K. Thomson, New York: Penguin Books. Bailin, S (1994). Achieving extraordinary ends: An essay on creativity. Norwood, N.J: Ablex. Bailin, S (1998). Education, knowledge and critical thinking. Education, knowledge and truth. D. Carr. London: Routledge: 204-221 BenHabib, S (1992). Situating the self: gender, community and postmodernism in contemporary ethics. Oxford: Polity Blagg, N (1991). Can We Teach Intelligence? A Comprehensive Evaluation of Feuerstein’s Instrumental Enrichment Program. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Blakemore, SJ and Frith, U (2000). The implications of recent developments in neuroscience for research on teaching and learning, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience. Boyle, T (1997) Design for multi-media learning. Hemel Hempstead: Prentice Hall Bromme, R and Stahl, E (2002) Writing and Learning: Hypertext as a renewal of an old and close friendship. In Bromme, R and Stahl, E (eds) Writing Hypertext and Learning: conceptual and empirical approaches. Advances in Learning and Instruction Series. Oxford: Pergamon 39 BIBLIOGRAPHY Burbules, N and Rice, S (1991). Dialogue across differences: continuing the conversation. Harvard Educational Review 61(4). Buzan, A and Buzan, B (2000). The Mind Map Book. London: BBC Carver, SM, Lehrer, R, Connell, T, & Ericksen, J. (1992). Learning by hypermedia design: Issues of assessment and implementation. Educational Psychologist, 27(3): 385-404. Clark, RE (1990). Facilitating domain-general prolem solving: Computers, cognitive processes and instruction. In E. De Corte, M. C. Linn, H. Mandl and L. Verschaffel (eds) Computer- Based Learning Environments and Problem Solving. Berlin: Springer-Verlag: 265-287. Claxton, G (1999). Wise-Up: the challenge of lifelong learning. London: Bloomsbury. Claxton, G and Wells, G Eds. (2002). Learning for life in the 21st Century. Oxford: Blackwell. Clements, DH and Gullo, DF (1984). Effects of computer programming on young children’s cognitions, Journal of Educational Psychology, 76: 1051-8. Cobb, P and McClain, K (2002). Supporting Students Learning of Significant Mathematical Ideas. In Wells, G. and Claxton, G (eds) Learning for life in the 21st Century. Coley, R, Cradler, J, & Engel, P (1997). Computers and classrooms: The status of technology in U.S. schools, p. 37. Princeton, NJ: Policy Information Center, Educational Testing Service. Craft, A (1991). Thinking skills and the whole curriculum. The Curriculum Journal 2( 2): 183-199. Crook, C (1994). Computers and the collaborative experience of learning. London and New York: Routledge. Dawes, L (1997). Teaching Talk. In Computers and Talk in the Primary Classroom, edited by R. Wegerif and P. Scrimshaw. Clevedon: Multi-lingual Matters. Dawes, L, Mercer, N and Wegerif, R (2000). Extending talking and reasoning skills using ICT. In Leask, M and Meadows (eds), J Teaching and Learning with ICT in the Primary School. London: Routledge Dawes, L, Wegerif, R and Mercer, N (2000). Thinking Together: Activities for Key Stage 2 children and teachers. London: Questions Publishing. De Bono, E (1976). Thinking Action: Teacher Handbook – CoRT VI. New York: Pergamon Press. De Corte, E (1990). Towards powerful learning environments for the acquisition of problem solving skills. European Journal of Psychology of Education 5: 5-19. De Corte, E, Linn, M Mandl, H & Verschaffel, L (Eds.) (1992). Computer-based learning environments and problem-solving. Berlin: Springer-Verlag: 247-262 Dewey, J (1933). How We Think 2e, New York: D. C. Heath DfES (2002)a The National Curriculum. London: HMSO. http://www.nc.uk.net/learn_think.html DfES (2002)b Impact2. www.becta.org.uk/research/impact2 Dillon, A and Gabbard, R (1998). Hypermedia as an educational technology: a review of the quantitative research literature on learning comprehension control and style. Review of educational research, 68: 322-349 Edwards, D (1996). Discourse and Cognition. London: Sage. Ennis, R (1996). Critical Thinking. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Facione, P (1990). Critical thinking: a statement of expert consensus for purposes of educational assessment and instruction. Millbrae, CA: California Academic Press. Fisher, R (1998). Teaching thinking. London: Cassell. FOLDOC (2002). Free Online Dictionary Of Computing (http://foldoc.doc.ic.ac.uk/ foldoc/index.html) Forrester, MA (1992). The Development of Young Children’s Social-Cognitive Skills, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 40 BIBLIOGRAPHY Graddol, D (1989). Some CMC discourse properties and their educational significance, in Mindweave, A. Kaye, Editor. Pergamon: Oxford. Greenfield, S (2000). Private life of the brain. Harmondsworth: Penguin. Greeno, J, Collins, A and Resnick, L (1996). Cognition and learning. Handbook of Educational Psychology. D. Berliner and R. Calfee. New York: MacMillan: 15-46. Habermas, J (1991). The Theory of Communicative Action Vol 1. Cambridge: Polity Press. Halliday, MAK and Martin, JR (1993). Writing Science: Literacy and Discursive Power, London, U.K: Falmer Press. Harasim, L et al., (1995). Learning Networks. Cambridge, Ma: MIT. Hasan, R (1992). Rationality in everyday talk: from process to system. In J Svartvik, (ed) Corpus Linguistics: Proceedings of Nobel Symposium 82, Stockholm 4-8. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Hattie, JA, Biggs, J and Purdie, N (1996). Effects of learning skills intervention on student learning: A meta-analysis. Review of Research in Education 66: 99-136. Hennessy, S, McCormick, R and Murphy, P (1993). The myth of general problem-solving capability: design and technology as an example. The Curriculum Journal 4(1). Higgins, S (2000). The Logical Zoombinis. Teaching Thinking. Vol. 1. Issue 1. Higgins, S (2001). ICT and Teaching for Understanding, Evaluation and Research in Education 15.3: 164-171 Higgins, S and Baumfield, V (1998). A defence of teaching general thinking skills. Journal of Philosophy of Education 32(3): 391-398. Hirsch, E (1987). Cultural Literacy: What every American Should Know. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Hobson, P (2002). The Cradle of Thought : Exploring the Origins of Thinking. London: Macmillan Hobson, RP (1998). The intersubjective foundations of thought. In S. Braten (ed.) Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Ontogeny. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 283-96 Hughes, M (1990). Children’s computation. In Understanding Children, edited by R. Grieve and M. Hughes. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Inkpen, K, Booth, KS, Klawe, M, and Upitis, R (1995). Playing Together Beats Playing Apart, Especially for Girls. Proceedings of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL) '95. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: 177-181. (http://www.cs.sfu.ca/people/Faculty/inkpen/pu blications.html.) Jermann, P, Soller, A, & Muehlenbrock, M. (2001). From Mirroring to Guiding: A Review of State of the Art Technology for Supporting Collaborative Learning. Proceedings of the First European Conference on Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, Maastricht, The Netherlands: 324-331. Johnston, S (2000). Teaching Thinking Skills. Impact 8: Report of the Philosophy of Education Society of Great Britain. ISBN 0-902221-09-2 Jonassen, D, Carr, C and Yueh, H (1998). Computers as Mindtools for Engaging Learners in Critical Thinking. TechTrends, v43 n2: 24-32 Jonassen, D (1991). Hypertext as instructional design. Educational Technology Research and Development, 39(1): 83-92 Jonassen, D (2000). Computers as mindtools for schools: engaging critical thinking. 2e. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Kirkwood, M (1998). A study of teaching and learning in computer education: designing an introductory programming course to foster understanding, problem solving, higher order thinking and metacognition. Unpublished PhD Glasgow, University of Glasgow. Kirkwood, M (2000). Infusing higher-order thinking and learning to learn into content instruction: a case study of secondary computing studies in Scotland. Journal of Curriculum Studies 32(4): 509-535. 41 Costa. Virginia, USA., Association of Supervision and Curriculum Development. Teasley, S and Roschelle, J (1993). Constructing a joint problem space: the computer as a tool for sharing knowledge. In Computers as Cognitive Tools, edited by S. LaJoie and S. Dery. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Underwood, J and Underwood, G (1990). Computers and Learning. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Underwood, J, Cavendish, S, Dowling, S, Fogelman, K. & Lawson, T (1996). Are integrated learning systems effective learning support tools? Computers & Education, 26 (1-3), 33-40. Van Boxtel, C, van der Linden, J and Kanselaar, G (2000). Collaborative learning tasks and the elaboration of conceptual knowledge Learning and Instruction 10: 311-330 Vygotsky, L (1986). Thought and Language. Cambridge Ma: M. I. T Press. Wegerif, R and Scrimshaw, P (1997). Computers and Talk in the Primary Classroom, Clevedon: Multi-lingual Matters. Wegerif, R, Sams, C and Barrett, G (2002). The role of computers in learning conversations. Paper at IFIP 2002 conference. Manchester. (http://fels-staff.open.ac.uk/ rupert-wegerif/rwpapers/WegerifIFIP.htm) Wegerif, R & Mercer, N (2000). Language for Thinking. In Cowie, H, Aalsvoort, D and Mercer, N. New Perspectives in Collaborative Learning. Oxford: Elsevier. http://fels-staff.open.ac.uk /rupert-wegerif/rwpapers.htm Wegerif, R (1996a). Collaborative learning and directive software, Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 12,1: 22-32. Wegerif, R (1996b). Using computers to help coach exploratory talk across the curriculum, Computers and Education, Vol 26, 1-3: 51 - 60. Wegerif, R (1997). Factors Affecting the Quality of Children's Talk at Computers. In Computers and Talk in the Primary Classroom, edited by R. Wegerif and P. Scrimshaw. Clevedon: Multi- lingual Matters. Wegerif, R (1998). The Social Dimension of Asynchronous Learning Networks. Journal of Asynchronous Learning Networks, Vol. 2, Issue 1. http://www.aln.org/alnweb/ journal/vol2_issue1/Wegerif.pdf ISSN 1092-8235 Wegerif, R, Mercer, N, and Dawes, L (1999). From social interaction to individual reasoning: an empirical investigation of a possible socio- cultural model of cognitive development. Learning and Instruction. Vol. 9, 5: 493-516 Wegerif, R, Mercer, N and Dawes, L (1998). Integrating Pedagogy and Software Design to Support Discussion in the Primary Curriculum. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 14: 199-211 Wegerif, R and Dawes, L (In press for 2003). Thinking and Learning with ICT: Raising achievement in the primary classroom. London: Routledge. Weinstein, M (1993). Critical thinking: the great debate. Educational Theory 43 (1). Wenger, E (1999). Communities of Practice. Learning, meaning and identity, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Whitebread, D (1997). Developing children's problem-solving: The educational uses of adventure games, in McFarlane, A. (ed) (1997) Information Technology and Authentic Learning, London: Routledge. Wilson, V (2000). Can Thinking Skills by Taught: A paper for discussion. Scottish Council for Research in Education, ( http://www.scre.ac.uk /scot-research/thinking/). Wittgenstein, L (1967). Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. 44 BIBLIOGRAPHY Acknowledgements Asking other people for help is perhaps the most effective but least commented upon thinking skill. I am very grateful to those who were kind enough to help me prepare this review. I would like to thank Steve Higgins and Keri Facer for their feedback on the first draft, Steve Higgins again and Margaret Kirkwood for providing me with material, and Julia Gillen, Richard Joiner, Peter Scrimshaw, Lyn Dawes and Charles Crook for helpful advice and references. The comments of all the other people I buttonholed at conferences to ask what they thought about technology and thinking are also acknowledged with thanks. About Futurelab Futurelab is passionate about transforming the way people learn. Tapping into the huge potential offered by digital and other technologies, we are developing innovative learning resources and practices that support new approaches to education for the 21st century. Working in partnership with industry, policy and practice, Futurelab: • incubates new ideas, taking them from the lab to the classroom • offers hard evidence and practical advice to support the design and use of innovative learning tools • communicates the latest thinking and practice in educational ICT • provides the space for experimentation and the exchange of ideas between the creative, technology and education sectors. A not-for-profit organisation, Futurelab is committed to sharing the lessons learnt from our research and development in order to inform positive change to educational policy and practice. Futurelab 1 Canons Road Harbourside Bristol BS1 5UH United Kingdom tel +44 (0)117 915 8200 fax +44 (0)117 915 8201
[email protected] www.futurelab.org.uk Registered charity 1113051 Creative Commons © Futurelab 2006. All rights reserved; Futurelab has an open access policy which encourages circulation of our work, including this report, under certain copyright conditions - however, please ensure that Futurelab is acknowledged. For full details of our Creative Commons licence, go to www.futurelab.org.uk/open_access.htm Disclaimer These reviews have been published to present useful and timely information and to stimulate thinking and debate. It should be recognised that the opinions expressed in this document are personal to the author and should not be taken to reflect the views of Futurelab. Futurelab does not guarantee the accuracy of the information or opinion contained within the review. This publication is available to download from the Futurelab website – www.futurelab.org.uk/research/lit_reviews.htm Also from Futurelab: Literature Reviews and Research Reports Written by leading academics, these publications provide comprehensive surveys of research and practice in a range of different fields. Handbooks Drawing on Futurelab's in-house R&D programme as well as projects from around the world, these handbooks offer practical advice and guidance to support the design and development of new approaches to education. Opening Education Series Focusing on emergent ideas in education and technology, this series of publications opens up new areas for debate and discussion. We encourage the use and circulation of the text content of these publications, which are available to download from the Futurelab website – www.futurelab.org.uk/research. For full details of our open access policy, go to www.futurelab.org.uk/open_access.htm.